“In the long history of humankind (and animal kind, too) those who learned to collaborate and improvise most effectively have prevailed,” wrote Darwin. Ants, a family that has inhabited the earth for about 100 million years, must be one of the most magnificent manifestations of such biological cooperation. Thousands of female ants pull together in a coordinated effort to ensure that all the needs are met for the proper functioning of the entire colony. (The males’ only role is to mate with the queen, and once this is completed, they die). While some ants forage, others stay behind to tend the brood, or to build, maintain or defend the colony’s living quarters; and there are even those whose task it is to bury the dead. But the thing that makes their behavior so remarkable is that they have no leader – no boss or governing body to allocate and manage their activities. How exactly do ants collaborate and divide the labor among themselves so successfully and altruistically?
In his new lab in the Physics of Complex Systems Department,
Dr. Ofer Feinerman and his team are hoping to reveal some of the ants’ secrets in a collaborative effort of their own, using tools from such fields as information theory, statistical and theoretical physics, computer science, systems biology, neuroscience and, of course biology. “Biology is rife with complex systems consisting of individual components – proteins, cells, organisms – organizing themselves into networks to coordinate their activity. While biology is able to identify and describe the individual components, the interactions between them can get very messy and analyzing such data can become overwhelming. By borrowing tools from physics and math, more quantitative measurements can be used to discern the rules that govern such complex collective behaviors,” says Feinerman.
Social networking
Ants primarily “speak” to one another in the language of chemicals: If an ant finds a rich food source, for example, it will deposit a trail of pheromones that tells the other ants where to find it. Social networks are formed between the ants during such communication, and it is these networks that Feinerman wants to understand.
In a setup reminiscent of Big Brother, Feinerman has handpicked a number of native Israeli ants to enter an artificial, nest-like structure that has cameras dotted around, enabling his team to eavesdrop on the ants’ “conversations.” Each ant is identified by a barcode glued to its back, enabling the scientists to track and record its activities. The scientists are hoping to answer such questions as: Who “speaks” to whom? Do ants form cliques and only interact with those in the same group, or are they indiscriminate? Do they employ far-reaching social networking tools akin to, say, Twitter or Facebook, relying on others to “retweet” and “share” their messages? Do messages get passed down a line of ants, or would an ant prefer to wander far and wide to make sure the message is relayed accurately?
Feinerman: “Ants use different communication strategies, ranging from one extreme to the other, depending on the setting. For example, in ‘piggybacking,’ one ant rides on top of another; though slow, this is a very reliable and direct method of communication, appropriate, for example, for ensuring the second ant arrives at the exact food location without getting lost along the way. Other situations may warrant less focused, but faster, forms of communication. For example, if a colony comes under attack, the ants spray pheromones into the air. This alarm system rapidly warns other, distant colonies of potentially imminent danger.”
Slow and reliable or fast with some compromise: Understanding ant communication and social networking strategies will do more than help reveal the intricate workings of an ant colony; they also assist in the long term goal of using this rich cooperative system to develop a theory of collective information processing. Ultimately, the researchers would like to develop the tools needed to answer fundamental questions about other complex biological systems: for instance, how immune system cells work together to fight infection. Such tools could also have practical applications in the design of so-called distributed systems, including cellular communication antennae, wireless sensor networks or even groups of robots engaged in rescue operations.
Empire of the Ants
At the head of each ant colony is a queen; but, though “royalty,” she does not possess any sovereign power over the worker ants – her sole duty is to lay eggs. With no ruler, how do ants divide labor? Do some ants belong to “elite” units, with others relegated to more “lower-class” duties? Or are they all equal?
In previous research, Dr. Feinerman investigated whether certain factors – previous experience, age, body weight, spatial location – determine how tasks are allocated. One way to test this is to see how ants respond to increased demands in certain tasks. For example, when food stores are running low, which ants take it upon themselves to aid in the foraging effort? Or offer a helping hand to nurse an extra brood in the colony?
The ants were placed in an artificial, two-chambered nest, and the scientists manipulated the task load by withholding food or adding new members to a brood. Again, the ants were individually tagged – this time using radio-frequency identification (RFID) – to identify individuals that responded.
This study revealed that it is the leaner ants that are usually on the frontline: They are the first to respond to increased foraging needs, even when other factors (age, experience and spatial location) are taken into account. They also seem to be the ones who engage in transporting the newly planted brood members to the main brood pile. However, lean ants do not seem to actually help in tending the brood. In fact, Feinerman found that brood care seems to be random, not dependent on by age, experience or weight, but rather, by whoever happens to be passing by.
“With regard to foraging, sending out the leaner ants could boost colony survival, as they would be more expendable in the risky task of foraging, may attract fewer predators, and might even be more mobile than their heavier counterparts. Likewise, the fact that brood care seems to be open to all suggests that flexibility in general task allocation, and collaboration on the whole, is more valuable than, say, expertise or experience, as it allows for a rapid response to a changing environment, thereby ensuring the survival of the colony,” says Feinerman.
ANT-thropologist
Dr. Ofer Feinerman was born in Rehovot, Israel, and he earned a B.Sc. in physics and mathematics
summa cum laude (1996) and an M.Sc. in physics (1999), both from the Hebrew University of Jerusalem. As a Ph.D. student at the Weizmann Institute of Science, under the guidance of
Prof. Elisha Moses, he grew the first artificial logic circuits made of nerve cells, earning his doctorate in 2006. He then conducted postdoctoral research for three years at the Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center in New York, investigating how the immune system's T-regulation cells work together to fight infection. Feinerman spent one year studying ants at Rockefeller University before joining the Department of Physics of Complex Systems at the Weizmann Institute of Science as a Senior Scientist in October 2010. “Coming back to the Weizmann Institute was a dream – not only did it give me the opportunity to return to Israel, but it also gave me the freedom to study ants – a somewhat unconventional subject. For this I am eternally grateful to and proud of the Weizmann Institute.”
Feinerman is married to Micka, a mosaic artist, and has three children: Matan (9), Shai (7) and Nomi (4).
Dr. Ofer Feinerman's research is supported by the Clore Foundation.
A Thing and Its Opposite
In his 1937 paper, Majorana took Dirac’s formula one step further, proposing that it is possible, in theory, to have an elementary particle that carries no electric charge; such a particle would, in fact, be its own antiparticle. This theoretical particle became known as Majorana’s fermion (a fermion being a particle of matter, rather than a force-carrying particle such as a photon).
Majorana’s personal life adds intrigue to the story: Although a gifted scientist who published his first scientific paper as an undergraduate, he was mentally unstable. His mentor, Enrico Fermi, called him a “genius of Newton’s rank.” But even as Fermi managed to convince him to publish his antiparticle paper, Majorana was becoming increasingly reclusive. Finally, in 1938, he boarded a ship in Palermo bound for Napoli. But he never disembarked. Although it is fairly certain that Majorana committed suicide, rumors of his having escaped to a monastery helped fuel the mystery surrounding the theory of a particle that is its own antiparticle.
Imaginary particles
The story picks up in 1982 with Robert Laughlin, an American physicist. Laughlin, in proposing an explanation for a quantum phenomenon called the Fractional Quantum Hall Effect, which occurs in semiconductors, made the claim that in certain circumstances imaginary particles – arising through the collective behavior of electrons – are formed and that these carry electric charges. Interestingly, the proposed charges were fractions of the basic charge of an electron.
This prediction was first proven experimentally in 1997 by the Weizmann Institute’s Prof. Moty Heiblum and his research group – an achievement that contributed to the decision to award Laughlin, Stormer and Tsui the Nobel Prize in Physics in 1998. But before long, other research into quantum phenomena began to hint at the existence of imaginary particles with completely different characteristics, those with fractional charges that were even – one-half, one-quarter, etc. Heiblum and his group succeeded in proving the existence of these charges, as well, a feat that depended on the growth of ultra-pure semiconductor crystals by the Institute’s Dr. Vladimir Umansky in the Braun Submicron Center.
Quantum switches
Imaginary particles come in two types: Abelian and non-Abelian. These mathematical terms, from the field of topology, refer to what happens when two particles in a system change places. Unlike the Abelian type, in the non-Abelian system (in which the particles have even denominators), this exchange moves the system from one quantum state to another. These quantum states are sensitive only to the topology of the path taken by the two particles in the exchange process.
Small Is beautiful
Another research path, which opened in 2007, began with an insight more common to sculpture than quantum physics: The level of detail that sculptors can achieve in their artwork is limited by the width of their finest carving tool. Past a certain physical point, the only way to introduce greater detail into the sculpture is to shape it from bottom up, by piecing together very tiny bits of stone. The dimensions of the smallest details will thus be determined by the size of the smallest stones.
States
The final path in this story began in 2001, with Prof. Alexei Kitaev, a former visiting scientist at the Weizmann Institute. Kitaev suggested that quantum topological memory could be created using a new version of non-Abelian imaginary particles – particles without any charge that would have the properties of Majorana particles. These would not be true Majorana’s fermions, which are particles of matter, but complex imaginary particles in which the lack of charge would be both a “state” and its own “anti-state.”
The paths converge
Oreg and his research student Yonatan Most had suggested that if Shtrikman’s nanowires were placed next to the superconductor, complex imaginary Majorana-like particles would appear at the ends of the wires. Heiblum, postdoctoral fellow Dr. Anindya Das and research student Yuval Ronen planned and built an experimental apparatus to test this theory, and within a few months they managed to find evidence for quantum states that fit the expected profile for the proposed Majorana states.
Open question
The quantum states seen at the Weizmann Institute and Delft via observations of the imaginary particles aroused much excitement. However, as noted, they are not true Majorana particles, which, as fermions, must be real matter particles to qualify. In other words, the question is still open: Do the particles that Majorana predicted actually exist in nature?
Some think that neutrinos could be Majorana particles, and if this is the case it might help solve one of the biggest mysteries in physics. In the Big Bang, equal amounts of matter and antimatter should have been produced, yet we observe only matter in our universe. How did matter survive in the world when all antimatter disappeared? If neutrinos were Majorana particles, this might imply an as-yet-undetected force in the universe that prefers matter over antimatter.